Warning: mkdir(): Permission denied in /home/virtual/lib/view_data.php on line 81 Warning: fopen(/home/virtual/audiology/journal/upload/ip_log/ip_log_2024-03.txt): failed to open stream: No such file or directory in /home/virtual/lib/view_data.php on line 83 Warning: fwrite() expects parameter 1 to be resource, boolean given in /home/virtual/lib/view_data.php on line 84 Academic Performance, Communication, and Psychosocial Development of Prelingual Deaf Children with Cochlear Implants in Mainstream Schools
J Audiol Otol Search

CLOSE


J Audiol Otol > Volume 24(2); 2020 > Article
Choi, Hong, and Moon: Academic Performance, Communication, and Psychosocial Development of Prelingual Deaf Children with Cochlear Implants in Mainstream Schools

Abstract

Background and Objectives

To assess the academic performance, communication skills, and psychosocial development of prelingual deaf children with cochlear implants (CIs) attending mainstream schools, and to evaluate the impact of auditory speech perception on their classroom performance.

Subjects and Methods

As participant, 67 children with CI attending mainstream schools were included. A survey was conducted using a structured questionnaire on academic performance in the native language, second language, mathematics, social studies, science, art, communication skills, self-esteem, and social relations. Additionally, auditory and speech performances on the last follow-up were reviewed retrospectively.

Results

Most implanted children attending mainstream school appeared to have positive self-esteem and confidence, and had little difficulty in conversing in a quiet classroom. Also, half of the implanted children (38/67) scored above average in general academic achievement. However, academic achievement in the second language (English), social studies, and science were usually poorer than general academic achievement. Furthermore, half of the implanted children had difficulty in understanding the class content (30/67) or conversing with peers in a noisy classroom (32/67). These difficulties were significantly associated with poor speech perception.

Conclusions

Improving the listening environment for implanted children attending mainstream schools is necessary.

Introduction

Profound deafness in childhood affects the development of auditory speech perception, speech production, and language skills. Failure to develop adequate oral communication skills can have a significant negative effect on academic performance and psychosocial development in these children. A cochlear implant (CI) is an electronic prosthetic device that is surgically placed in the inner ear to provide useful sound perception by electrically stimulating the auditory nerve. It has been proved to be an effective management device for children with bilaterally severe-to-profound sensorineural hearing loss receiving limited benefit from hearing aids (HAs). Many studies have demonstrated the benefits of CIs concerning speech perception, production, and language development due to the enhanced audition provided by CIs [1-5]. Thus, the academic performance and psychosocial development of children with CIs is expected to be better than their non-implanted peers.
Over the past two to three decades, there has been a significant trend towards placing students with special educational needs in mainstream schools rather than in segregated special schools and special classes [6]. Besides, the proportion of children with CIs in mainstream schools has been steadily increasing [7-9]. Thus, classroom performance and social integration are some of the major challenges faced by children with CIs in mainstream schools. However, only a few studies have addressed the academic performance and psychosocial development of these children in mainstream schools, and have reported mixed results. Some studies reported that children with CIs in mainstream schools had satisfactory academic achievement [10,11], while others reported that their academic performance lagged behind their normal-hearing peers [12,13]. Information on the subject-specific academic performance of these children is also lacking [13,14]. As with academic performance, wide variability in psychosocial development has been reported. Some studies reported positive findings regarding the social well-being and social functioning of children with CIs in mainstream schools [12,15,16] while others revealed ongoing difficulties in social participation with normal-hearing peers [17-19].
For successful social integration of children with CIs in inclusive educational settings, the overall classroom performance of children with CIs in mainstream schools, and the factors affecting this performance need to be determined. Thus, our study aimed to assess the academic performance, communication skills, and psychosocial development of prelingual deaf children with CIs attending mainstream schools, and to evaluate the impact of auditory speech perception on their classroom performance. Given the lack of evaluation tools, we used structured questionnaires to examine academic performance in several subjects, communication skills, self-esteem, and the social relations of implanted children. We also reviewed medical speech evaluation records retrospectively.

Subjects and Methods

Participants

A prospective cohort study of prelingual deaf children with CIs was performed from August to September 2015. All children underwent CI before the age of 5 years at Samsung Medical Center and had at least 5 years of experience with the CI. Of the 149 children, 72 agreed to participate in the in-person or telephone interview along with their parents. Of the 72 children, 67 children attending mainstream schools were enrolled. Participants included 30 boys and 37 girls with an age range of 6 to 17 years [mean±standard deviation (SD)=10±3 years] at the time of the study. Before implantation, CI candidates underwent auditory brainstem response and auditory steadystate response to predict their hearing thresholds. All the children had bilateral profound sensorineural hearing loss (>90 dB) across the speech frequency range and showed limited benefit from HAs in best-aided conditions. Fifty-one children were implanted with the Nucleus device (Cochlear Corporation, Lane Cove, New South Wales, Australia), and 16 children were implanted with the Clarion device (Advance Bionics Corporation, Sylmar, CA, USA). First devices were implanted at an age ranging from 13 months to 4 years 10 months (mean± SD=31±13 months). The duration of implant usage ranged from 5 to 13 years (mean±SD=8±2 years).

Ethics statement

All the participants were recruited and assessed in the Hearing Laboratory at the Samsung Medical Center. Written informed consent for participation was taken from each participant. The Institutional Review Board of Samsung Medical Center (IRB No. 2015-08-126-007) approved this study. All the study protocols were performed following the relevant guidelines and regulations.

Auditory and speech performance evaluation

Auditory and speech performances on the last follow-up at the Samsung Medical Center, at least 3 years after the implantation, were reviewed retrospectively. Aided pure-tone thresholds were measured in the sound field using warble tones presented from a loudspeaker located one meter away from the children at 0 degrees azimuth by an experienced audiologist [20]. The auditory perception was evaluated by the categories of auditory performance (CAP) scores. CAP comprises 8 hierarchical divisions of auditory perceptive ability: 0, the lowest level, describes no awareness of environmental sounds; and 7, the highest level, describes the use of a telephone with a familiar speaker. Speech perception in both auditory-only (AO) and audiovisual conditions was evaluated using consonant-vowel-consonant monosyllabic and bi-syllabic words. Stimuli were presented using a monitored live voice of an experienced speech therapist. The presentation level was approximately 70 dB SPL, as determined with a handheld sound level meter. Children were instructed to repeat each presented stimulus. Speech acquisition and production were assessed using the speech production scale (SPC) and the modification of Ling’s 7 distinct stages [21,22] of the normal data of Korean phoneme development. The SPC assesses phonetic (articulation) and phonologic (meaningful speech) acquisition levels at each stage in young children with CI. The ‘staircase’ diagram of SPC is illustrated in in Supplementary Material 1 (in the online-only Data Supplement).

Questionnaires

A survey of academic performance, communication skills, and psychosocial development were conducted through inperson or telephone interviews. A structured questionnaire was designed for children and their parents. Background information about device strategies (unilateral, bilateral, and bimodal CI users) and usage, communication modes (oral or/ and sign language), and school type were evaluated. Five categories of school enrollment were recognized: ordinary classes in mainstream schools, special classes in mainstream schools, special schools, schools for the deaf, and vocational schools. Grade retention, defined as the need to repeat a year due to failing grades, was also assessed. Academic achievement was assessed using quartiles and included the survey of overall academic performance, language (Korean and English), mathematics, social studies, science, and artistic performance (art and music). Communication development was assessed using closed questions on various situations at school and at home. Lastly, self-esteem and social relations were assessed by inquiring about friends and future hopes. An English translation of the questionnaire is provided in Supplementary Material 2 (in the online-only Data Supplement).

Statistical analysis

Results were analyzed using SPSS 18.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). Clinical information (age, sex, side of first CI, age at first CI, and duration of first CI use) and the results of auditory and speech performance evaluations were compared between the three listening modes using the Kruskal-Wallis and the chi-square tests. In case of a significant difference between the three listening modes, a post-hoc analysis using the Mann-Whitney test or chi-square test was performed to evaluate the differences between two different listening modes (i.e., unilateral CI users vs. bimodal listeners, unilateral CI users vs. bilateral CI users, and bimodal listeners vs. bilateral CI users). Chi-square test or Fisher’s exact test was used to compare the distribution of responses to items on academic achievement, communication skills, and psychosocial development according to the three listening modes. To evaluate the impact of auditory speech perception ability on classroom performance, a multivariate logistic model was used to compare the bi-syllabic word recognition scores (WRSs) in AO condition and classroom performance after adjusting for the current age, sex, and listening modes. The Bonferroni correction was applied to the p-value because of multiple testing. Adjusted p-values were calculated by multiplying the original p-values by the number of comparisons (3 for listening modes). A p-value or adjusted p-value of less than 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Results

Demographics

Among 67 children with CIs, 17 children (25%) wore only one CI (unilateral CI users), 29 children (43%) were experienced bimodal listeners with a CI in one ear and an HA in the other (HA+CI), and 21 children (31%) received two implants in sequential procedures (bilateral CI users, CI+CI) with the interval between the first and second implantations ranging from 1-8 years (mean±SD interval=3±2 years). All the children reportedly wore their device(s) more than 70% of the day and used oral communication. Supplementary Table 1 (in the online-only Data Supplement) includes the clinical and device information of each implanted children. Table 1 presents the comparison of the clinical information between the three listening modes (unilateral CI user, bimodal listener, and bilateral CI user). No significant difference was found in age and first CI information (side of first CI, age at first CI, and duration of first CI use) between the three listening modes. However, the sex ratio was significantly different between the three listening modes (p=0.035).
Table 1 also presents the comparisons of the auditory and speech performances between the three listening modes. No significant difference was found in aided pure-tone thresholds of the first device between the three listening modes. Supplementary Table 2 (in the online-only Data Supplement) includes the individual aided pure-tone thresholds at 250, 500 Hz, 1, 2, and 4 kHz. Most children showed good performance in speech evaluation 3 years after the implantation. However, there were significant differences in CAP scores (p=0.006), WRSs (p<0.05 for monosyllabic and bi-syllabic), and SPSs (p=0.001 for phonetic level and p=0.002 for phonologic level) between the three different modes. Overall, the bimodal listeners and bilateral CI users showed significantly superior auditory perception (CAP scores and WRSs) and superior speech production in post-hoc analysis than unilateral CI users (Table 1). There were no significant differences in auditory perception and speech production between bimodal listeners and bilateral CI users in the post-hoc analysis.

Academic performance

Of the total 67 implanted children, 61 and 6 children were enrolled in ordinary classes with normal-hearing peers and a special class in a mainstream school, respectively. Grade retentions were reportedly experienced by 12 children (18%) one or more times. Table 2 shows participants’ clinical information and speech performance. Eight of these children were unilateral CI users, 1 child was a bimodal listener, and 3 were bilateral CI users. Two children had Noonan syndrome, 1 child had congenital heart disease, and 1 child had enlarged vestibular aqueduct syndrome.
Fig. 1A shows the academic performance of the 67 implanted children attending mainstream schools in each subject. When the academic achievement was assessed using quartiles, 28, 10, 17, and 12 implanted children had general academic achievement in the 1-25% (top 25%), 26-50%, 51-75%, and 76-100% (bottom 25%) of their class, respectively. Academic achievements in English (p=0.005 in chi-square test) and social studies (p=0.039 in chi-square test) were significantly lower than general academic achievement. Fig. 1B shows the academic performance in each subject according to the listening modes. Academic achievement in social studies (p=0.036 in Fischer’s exact test) and science (p=0.028 in Fischer’s exact test) differed significantly according to the listening modes. In post-hoc analysis with Fischer’s exact test, unilateral CI users showed poorer academic achievement in social studies (adjusted p=0.026) and science (adjusted p=0.014) than bilateral CI users. These differences were insignificant between bimodal listeners and bilateral CI users.

Communication skills and psychosocial development

Table 3 shows communication skills at school and at home, and psychosocial development of the 67 implanted children attending mainstream schools. About half (55%) of the children understood all or almost all of the teaching content (Q1 at school). In a quiet classroom, most implanted children (79%) reported little or no difficulty during one-on-one conversations (Q2 at school). Also, most implanted children (88%) experienced little or no stress conversing with an unfamiliar person at school (Q4 at school). However, half of the implanted children (48%) reported some difficulty during individual conversations in a noisy classroom (Q3 at school). Difficulty in communicating during class (Q1 at school) or in a quiet classroom (Q2 at school) differed significantly depending on the listening mode. In post-hoc analysis with Fischer’s exact test, bilateral CI users understood the class content better than bimodal listeners (adjusted p=0.037) and were more comfortable with individual conversations in a quiet classroom than unilateral CI users (adjusted p=0.02).
Implanted children generally experienced more difficulty communicating at school than at home. At home, 91% and 76% of implanted children reported little or no difficulty communicating in a quiet and noisy background, respectively (Q1 and Q2 at home). However, only 34% of the implanted children were able to understand all or almost all the words of their family while concentrating on other things (Q3 at home). No significant difference was found in communication skills at home between the three listening modes.
A survey of psychosocial development revealed that most implanted children (96%) had a friend with normal hearing (Q1), and 96% of implanted children were unafraid of making a new friend (Q5). When asked about their future careers, 78% of the children wanted a job (Q2) in various fields regardless of their communication skills. Furthermore, 91% and 90% of the implanted children responded positively when asked about getting a job (Q3) and performing workplace tasks (Q4) in the future. No significant difference was seen in the responses to the psychosocial development survey between the three listening modes.

The impact of auditory speech performance on classroom performance

Bivariate logistic regression models were constructed to examine the relationship between bi-syllabic WRS in AO condition and classroom performance (Table 4). Classroom performance (academic achievement and communication at school) was divided into good and poor performance based on the questionnaire responses. Good performers were defined as children who scored above average (within the top 50%) on the academic achievement questionnaires, or to the top 1 and 2 on the communication skills questionnaires. The remaining were defined as children with poor performance. A multivariate logistic regression model was constructed while adjusting for potential confounders (current age, sex, and listening modes). In the crude model, high scores on the bi-syllabic WRSs were significantly associated with a decrease in the odds of having poor academic performance in all subjects except art and music (all p<0.05). However, academic achievement in art and music was not significantly associated with the bi-syllabic WRSs. In the adjusted model, the bi-syllabic identification scores were significantly associated with academic achievement in Korean, social studies, and science (all p<0.05). High scores on the bi-syllabic WRSs were also significantly associated with a decrease in the odds of having communication difficulties at school (all p<0.05 in the adjusted model).

Discussion

In this study, most implanted children (61 of 67) attended ordinary classes with normal-hearing peers in a mainstream school. In Korea, students requiring special education are usually placed in mainstream schools rather than in separate special schools and special classes. Previous studies have emphasized the following principal benefits of inclusive education of implanted children: naturalistic access to typical linguistic and behavioral models of hearing peers, and social acceptance by hearing peers [6,23,24]. Children with CIs in mainstream schools reported having friends with mostly normal hearing (Q1 in Table 3). This result reflects their social interactions with peers during school life. Thus, the inclusive education of implanted children in mainstream schools appears to provide opportunities for contact and social interactions with normal peers. Based on the responses to the psychosocial development questions (self-esteem and social relations section in Table 3), most implanted children in mainstream schools seemed to have a positive attitude towards self-esteem and confidence.
In a mainstream education setting, half the implanted children scored above average in general academic achievement (Fig. 1A). Consistent with a previous study [13], academic achievement was best in mathematics. However, academic achievement in a second language (English) and social studies were usually poorer than general academic achievement (Fig. 1A). Particularly, our cohort reported difficulty in listening comprehension and pronunciation in English. When interviewed, the children also had difficulty understanding abstract concepts in social studies and science. Children with lower scores on the bi-syllabic identification test were significantly likely to have poor academic performance in social studies and science (Table 4). This may be because these subjects are often taught with complex verbal explanations. Children with CI have been reported to experience delays in concept formation [25]. Therefore, it is necessary to provide these children with various supplementary materials or preteach them the subject-relevant vocabulary so that the new information can be integrated meaningfully into the prior knowledge framework.
Most implanted children had little difficulty in conversing in a quiet background at school or home (Table 3). However, communication at school was considerably more challenging than at home in a noisy background. In a noisy background, 24% of the implanted children responded having some difficulty communicating at home, while half of the implanted children (48%) had some difficulty conversing at school. Also, half of the implanted children (45%) had difficulty in understanding the class. Although children with CIs can perceive speech, they may have difficulty in understanding during group or outdoor activities. Therefore, providing a good listening environment by considering the seating arrangements or using hearing assistive technologies such as personal FM systems for some children with poor speech perception is necessary for improving classroom performance.
This study had several limitations. First, the academic performance outcomes are mostly derived from parents’ opinions rather than from scores on validated measures directly testing the children’s outcomes. Careful observation of implanted children’s classroom performance is required in both national and school-based achievement testing for tracking studies. Second, this study included a heterogeneous group of children in terms of age, implantation age, CI device characteristics, presence or absence of additional disabilities, and the range of received educational supports. This study did not consider the CI device characteristics or educational supports. However, when adjusted for current age, sex, and listening modes, this study demonstrated that speech performance was significantly associated with academic performance. Educating children with CIs in mainstream schools appears to positively impact self-esteem and confidence in social integration in the future. However, 12 of the 67 implanted children attending mainstream schools experienced one or more grade retention episodes. The academic performance of CI children in mainstream schools was mostly satisfactory; however, they scored below average in some subjects. Furthermore, they reported some difficulty in understanding the class content and holding a conversation in a noisy background. Most of these difficulties were reflected by poor speech perception. Therefore, improving the listening environment for implanted children in mainstream schools is necessary.

Supplementary Material

The online-only Data Supplement is available with this article at (https://doi.org/10.7874/jao.2019.00346).

Supplementary Material 1

Staircase Diagram of the SPC
jao-2019-00346suppl1.pdf

Supplementary Material 2

English Version of the Questionnaires
jao-2019-00346suppl2.pdf

Supplementary Table 1

Clinical and device information of individual implanted children
jao-2019-00346suppl3.pdf

Supplementary Table 2

Aided pure-tone thresholds of implanted children
jao-2019-00346suppl4.pdf

Acknowledgments

None

Notes

Conflicts of interest

The authors have no financial conflicts of interest.

Author contributions

Conceptualization: Sung Hwa Hong and Il Joon Moon. Data curation: Ji Eun Choi. Formal analysis: Ji Eun Choi. Methodology: Il Joon Moon. Project administration: Sung Hwa Hong. Supervision: Il Joon Moon. Visualization: Ji Eun Choi. Writing—original draft: Ji Eun Choi. Writing—review & editing: Ji Eun Choi and Il Joon Moon. Approval of final manuscript: All authors.

Fig. 1.
Academic performance according to subject (A) and listening mode (B). The academic achievement of each subject is presented as a proportion of four quartiles. The upper quartile (1-25%) refers to the top 25% of students, and the lower quartile (76-100%) the bottom 25%. A p-value (p) less than 0.05 was considered to indicate statistical significance. CI: cochlear implant, HA: hearing aid.
jao-2019-00346f1.jpg
Table 1.
Demographics of unilateral CI users, bimodal listeners, and bilateral CI users
Variable Group 1 (n=17)
Group 2 (n=29)
Group 3 (n=21)
p-values p* for group 1 vs. 2 p* for group 1 vs. 3 p* for group 2 vs. 3
Unilateral CI users Bimodal listeners Bilateral CI users
Age (years) 11±3 10±3 10±3 0.118§
Sex (male:female) 11:6 8:21 11:10 0.035 0.042 1.332 0.225
Side of first device (right:left) 9:8 14:15 16:5 0.125
First device (Nucleus:Clarion) 10:7 23:6 18:3 0.134
Age at first CI (months) 30±13 31±11 30±15 0.669§
Duration of first CI use (years) 9±3 8±2 8±2 0.057§
Aided PTA of the first CI (W4FA, dB HL) 27±6 28±11 25±4 0.538§
CAP scores 5.8±1.2 6.6±0.7 6.7±0.7 0.006§ 0.018 0.021 2.424
Mono-syllabic WRSs (%)
 AV condition 80±32 96±19 99±5 <0.001§ 0.003 <0.001 0.951
 AO condition 61±31 86±19 93±8 <0.001§ <0.001 <0.001 0.231
Bi-syllabic WRSs (%)
 AV condition 85±32 96±19 99±5 0.009§ 0.066 0.027 0.951
 AO condition 68±33 89±19 95±8 <0.001§ 0.018 <0.001 0.165
Speech production scale
 Phonetic level 6.3±1.3 7.3±0.9 7.6±0.7 0.001§ 0.012 0.003 0.882
 Phonologic level 6.6±1.2 7.4±0.9 7.7±0.6 0.002§ 0.045 0.003 0.345

Continuous values were presented as mean±standard deviation.

* Adjusted p-values were calculated by multiplying the original p-values by 3 pairs,

chi-squared test,

Mann–Whitney test,

§ Kruskal-Wallis test,

p<0.05.

CI: cochlear implant, PTA: pure-tone average, W4FA: weighted four-frequency average [W4FA=(0.5 kHz+2×1 kHz+2×2 kHz+4 kHz)⁄6], CAP: categories of auditory performance, AV: audiovisual, AO: auditory-only, WRS: word recognition score

Table 2.
Clinical information and speech performance of children who experienced grade retention
Subject Sex Current age (year) First Cochlear Implantation
Aided PTA W4F (dB HL) Bi-syllabic WRS in AO condition (%) Comments
Type of device Age at implantation (month) Duration of CI use (year)
UCI5 M 15 N 50 12 28.3 60 Congenital heart disease
UCI6 M 14 C 36 11 35.0 0
UCI11 M 10 N 24 9 18.3 92
UCI12 F 12 C 39 9 30.8 0
UCI14 F 14 N 47 10 36.7 36
UCI15 F 12 C 35 10 23.3 80
UCI16 M 11 C 23 10 25.0 76 Revision CI (3 years after CI)
UCI17 M 7 N 32 5 29.2 84
CI+HA12 F 9 N 27 7 22.5 84 Noonan syndrome
BCI2 F 17 N 58 12 27.5 88
BCI4 F 9 N 21 8 22.5 100 EVAS
BCI18 M 8 C 18 7 26.7 100 Noonan syndrome

M: male, F: female, N: Nucleus, C: Clarion, CI: cochlear implant, PTA: pure-tone average, W4F: weighted four-frequency average [W4F=(0.5 kHz+2×1 kHz+2×2 kHz+4 kHz)⁄6], EVAS: enlarged vestibular aqueduct syndrome, WRS: word recognition score, AO: auditory-only, UCI: unilateral CI, HA: hearing aid, BCI: bilateral CI

Table 3.
Communication skills and psychosocial development
Questionnaire Total Unilateral CI users Bimodal listeners Bilateral CI users p for groups
Communication at school
Q1. In the class, 0.004*
A1 I can understand all the teaching content. 13 2 2 9
A2 I can understand almost all the teaching content. 24 5 12 7
A3 I can understand about half of the teaching content. 25 6 14 5
A4 I can understand only 20-30% of the teaching content. 4 4 0 0
A5 I barely understand the teaching content. 1 0 0 0
Q2. During a one-on-one conversation with a friend in a quiet classroom, 0.033*
A1 I have no difficulty having a conversation. 37 9 14 14
A2 I have a little difficulty having a conversation. 16 1 9 6
A3 I can understand about half of my friend’s words. 11 6 5 0
A4 I can understand 20-30% of my friend’s words. 3 1 1 1
A5 I struggle to have a conversation. 0 0 0 0
Q3. During a one-on-one conversation with a friend in a noisy classroom, 0.382
A1 I have no difficulty having a conversation. 4 0 1 3
A2 I have a little difficulty having a conversation. 31 7 13 11
A3 I can understand about half of my friend’s words. 25 6 13 6
A4 I can understand 20-30% of my friend’s words. 5 3 1 1
A5 I struggle to have a conversation. 2 1 1 0
Q4. During conversation with an unfamiliar person, 0.968
A1 I can converse without stress. 29 8 11 10
A2 I am slightly stressed during the conversation, but I can still talk. 30 7 14 9
A3 I try to avoid the conversation as much as possible. 8 2 4 2
A4 I absolutely avoid the conversation. 0 0 0 0
Communication at home
Q1. During conversation with family in a quiet background, 0.664
A1 I have no difficulty having a conversation. 52 11 24 17
A2 I have a little difficulty having a conversation. 9 4 3 2
A3 I can understand about half of my family’s words. 6 2 2 2
A4 I can understand 20-30% of my family’s words. 0 0 0 0
A5 I struggle to have a conversation. 0 0 0 0
Q2. During conversation with family in a noisy background, 0.786
A1 I have no difficulty having a conversation. 23 4 11 8
A2 I have a little difficulty having a conversation. 28 7 11 10
A3 I can understand about half of my family’s words. 12 5 5 2
A4 I can understand 20-30% of my family’s words. 4 1 2 1
A5 I struggle to have a conversation. 0 0 0 0
Q3. If my family suddenly speaks to me while I am concentrating on other things (e.g., watching TV, reading a book), 0.814
A1 I can understand all the words. 4 0 1 3
A2 I can understand almost all the words. 19 5 8 6
A3 Sometimes I ask what was said because I have missed words. 32 8 16 8
A4 I usually ask what was said because I have missed words. 9 3 3 3
A5 I miss words spoken by my family. 3 1 1 1
Self-esteem and social relations
Q1. My friends 0.784
A1 Mostly have normal hearing. 64 17 27 20
A2 Mostly have hearing loss and use hearing aids or cochlear implants. 0 0 0 0
A3 Range from those with normal hearing to those with hearing loss. 3 0 2 1
A4 Mostly have hearing loss and use sign language. 0 0 0 0
Q2. What do you want to be (do) in the future? 0.332
A1 I want to be a (      ). 52 11 24 17
A2 I don’t know. 15 6 5 4
Q3. Do you think that you’ll be able to have the same career as people with normal hearing? 0.299
A1 Yes, I think so. 37 7 15 15
A2 It may be difficult, but I think it will be possible if I try. 24 7 12 5
A3 I think it’s very unlikely. 6 3 2 1
A4 I think it’s impossible. 0 0 0 0
Q4. Do you think it will be difficult to perform tasks after getting a job because of difficulty communicating? 0.220
A1 No, I don’t anticipate that this will be an issue as I have no communication difficulties. 26 4 15 7
A2 There may be some communication difficulties, but it will not have a significant impact on performing tasks. 34 9 13 12
A3 I think communication difficulties will affect my work. I think it will be difficult to perform most of the tasks required of me. 5 3 1 1
A4 I don’t think I will be able to carry out my work because of communication difficulties. 2 1 0 1
Q5. When I make a new friend, 0.404
A1 I am not afraid to make a new friend because I don’t have any difficulty communicating with new people. 40 8 19 13
A2 Even though I have a little difficulty communicating with new people, I am not afraid to make a new friend. 24 7 9 8
A3 I am afraid to make a new friend because it is difficult for me to communicate with new people. 3 2 1 0
A4 I don’t want to make a new friend because I can’t communicate with new people. 0 0 0 0

* p<0.05,

Fischer’s exact test,

chi-squared test.

CI: cochlear implant

Table 4.
Relationship among bi-syllabic word recognition and classroom performance
Variables Good (ref) vs. poor performers Bi-syllabic WRS (%)
Crude model
Adjusted model*
N Mean±SD OR 95% CI OR 95% CI
Academic achievement
 General 1-50% 38 94±7 reference
51-100% 29 75±32 0.939 (0.892-0.987) 0.948 (0.898-1.001)
 Korean 1-50% 43 92±10 reference
51-100% 24 74±35 0.961 (0.930-0.993) 0.958 (0.922-0.996)
 English 1-50% 24 94±8 reference
51-100% 43 81±28 0.940 (0.883-1.000) 0.941 (0.879-1.008)
 Mathematics 1-50% 40 94±7 reference
51-100% 27 74±33 0.940 (0.896-0.987) 0.950 (0.902-1.001)
 Social studies 1-50% 32 94±9 reference
51-100% 35 79±30 0.952 (0.909-0.996) 0.924 (0.867-0.985)
 Science 1-50% 30 94±7 reference
51-100% 37 79±30 0.947 (0.900-0.996) 0.926 (0.868-0.988)
 Art and music 1-50% 41 91±17 reference
51-100% 26 78±30 0.976 (0.952-1.001) 0.981 (0.955-1.009)
Communications at school
 Q1 A1-2 37 94±6 reference
A3-5 30 75±32 0.924 (0.872-0.980) 0.925 (0.868-0.986)
 Q2 A1-2 53 93±8 reference
A3-5 14 59±40 0.929 (0.885-0.977) 0.937 (0.888-0.989)
 Q3 A1-2 35 94±6 reference
A3-5 32 77±31 0.939 (0.892-0.990) 0.930 (0.877-0.987)
 Q4 A1-2 59 87±21 reference
A3-4 8 75±37 0.984 (0.960-1.008) 0.881 (0.786-0.987)

Good performers were defined as children who scored above average (within the top 50%) on the academic achievement questionnaires, or among the top 1 and 2 on the questionnaires on communication skills. The rest of the children were defined as children with poor performance.

* A multivariate logistic regression model was constructed adjusting for current age, sex, and listening modes. The bi-syllabic WRSs were measured in auditory only conditions,

p<0.05.

WRS: word recognition score, N: Nucleus, SD: standard deviation, OR: odds ratio, CI: confidence interval

REFERENCES

1. Svirsky MA, Robbins AM, Kirk KI, Pisoni DB, Miyamoto RT. Language development in profoundly deaf children with cochlear implants. Psychol Sci 2000;11:153–8.
crossref pmid pmc
2. Richter B, Eissele S, Laszig R, Löhle E. Receptive and expressive language skills of 106 children with a minimum of 2 years’ experience in hearing with a cochlear implant. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 2002;64:111–25.
crossref pmid
3. Calmels MN, Saliba I, Wanna G, Cochard N, Fillaux J, Deguine O, et al. Speech perception and speech intelligibility in children after cochlear implantation. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 2004;68:347–51.
crossref pmid
4. Connor CM, Craig HK, Raudenbush SW, Heavner K, Zwolan TA. The age at which young deaf children receive cochlear implants and their vocabulary and speech-production growth: is there an added value for early implantation? Ear Hear 2006;27:628–44.
crossref pmid
5. Nicholas JG, Geers AE. Effects of early auditory experience on the spoken language of deaf children at 3 years of age. Ear Hear 2006;27:286–98.
crossref pmid pmc
6. Xie YH, Potměšil M, Peters B. Children who are deaf or hard of hearing in inclusive educational settings: a literature review on interactions with peers. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ 2014;19:423–37.
crossref pmid pdf
7. Francis HW, Koch ME, Wyatt JR, Niparko JK. Trends in educational placement and cost-benefit considerations in children with cochlear implants. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 1999;125:499–505.
crossref pmid
8. Punch R, Hyde M. Children with cochlear implants in Australia: educational settings, supports, and outcomes. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ 2010;15:405–21.
crossref pmid pdf
9. Geers A, Brenner C. Background and educational characteristics of prelingually deaf children implanted by five years of age. Ear Hear 2003;24:2S–14S.
crossref pmid
10. Wu CM, Liu TC, Liao PJ, Chen CK, Chang BL, Lin BG. Academic achievements and classroom performance in Mandarin-speaking prelingually deafened school children with cochlear implants. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 2013;77:1474–80.
crossref pmid
11. Motasaddi-Zarandy M, Rezai H, Mahdavi-Arab M, Golestan B. The scholastic achievement of profoundly deaf children with cochlear implants compared to their normal peers. Arch Iran Med 2009;12:441–7.
pmid
12. Damen GW, van den Oever-Goltstein MH, Langereis MC, Chute PM, Mylanus EA. Classroom performance of children with cochlear implants in mainstream education. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol 2006;115:542–52.
crossref pmid
13. Mukari SZ, Ling LN, Ghani HA. Educational performance of pediatric cochlear implant recipients in mainstream classes. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 2007;71:231–40.
crossref pmid
14. Spencer LJ, Gantz BJ, Knutson JF. Outcomes and achievement of students who grew up with access to cochlear implants. Laryngoscope 2004;114:1576–81.
crossref pmid pmc
15. Wauters LN, Knoors H. Social integration of deaf children in inclusive settings. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ 2008;13:21–36.
crossref pmid pdf
16. Schorr EA. Early cochlear implant experience and emotional functioning during childhood: Loneliness in middle and late childhood. Volta Review 2006;106:365–79.
crossref
17. Punch R, Hyde M. Social participation of children and adolescents with cochlear implants: a qualitative analysis of parent, teacher, and child interviews. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ 2011;16:474–93.
crossref pmid pdf
18. Martin D, Bat-Chava Y, Lalwani A, Waltzman SB. Peer relationships of deaf children with cochlear implants: predictors of peer entry and peer interaction success. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ 2011;16:108–20.
crossref pmid pdf
19. Bat-Chava Y, Deignan E. Peer relationships of children with cochlear implants. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ 2001;6:186–99.
crossref pmid pdf
20. Carhart R, Jerger JF. Preferred method for clinical determination of pure-tone thresholds. J Speech Hear Disord 1959;24:330–45.
crossref
21. Ling D. Speech development in hearing-impaired children. J Commun Disord 1978;11:119–24.
crossref pmid
22. Moon IJ, Kim EY, Chu H, Chung WH, Cho YS, Hong SH. A new measurement tool for speech development based on Ling’s stages of speech acquisition in pediatric cochlear implant recipients. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 2011;75:495–9.
crossref pmid
23. Percy-Smith L, Jensen JH, Cayé-Thomasen P, Thomsen J, Gudman M, Lopez AG. Factors that affect the social well-being of children with cochlear implants. Cochlear Implants Int 2008;9:199–214.
crossref pmid
24. van Gent T, Goedhart AW, Hindley PA, Treffers PD. Prevalence and correlates of psychopathology in a sample of deaf adolescents. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2007;48:950–8.
crossref pmid
25. Castellanos I, Kronenberger WG, Beer J, Colson BG, Henning SC, Ditmars A, et al. Concept formation skills in long-term cochlear implant users. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ 2015;20:27–40.
crossref pmid pdf


ABOUT
ARTICLES

Browse all articles >

ISSUES
TOPICS

Browse all articles >

AUTHOR INFORMATION
Editorial Office
The Catholic University of Korea, Institute of Biomedical Industry, 4017
222, Banpo-daero, Seocho-gu, Seoul, Republic of Korea
Tel: +82-2-3784-8551    Fax: +82-0505-115-8551    E-mail: jao@smileml.com                

Copyright © 2024 by The Korean Audiological Society and Korean Otological Society. All rights reserved.

Developed in M2PI

Close layer
prev next