Warning: mkdir(): Permission denied in /home/virtual/lib/view_data.php on line 87 Warning: chmod() expects exactly 2 parameters, 3 given in /home/virtual/lib/view_data.php on line 88 Warning: fopen(/home/virtual/audiology/journal/upload/ip_log/ip_log_2024-12.txt): failed to open stream: No such file or directory in /home/virtual/lib/view_data.php on line 95 Warning: fwrite() expects parameter 1 to be resource, boolean given in /home/virtual/lib/view_data.php on line 96 Asymmetry of the Subjective Visual Vertical in Patients With Unilateral Peripheral Vestibular Deficit

Asymmetry of the Subjective Visual Vertical in Patients With Unilateral Peripheral Vestibular Deficit

Article information

J Audiol Otol. 2024;28(3):213-220
Publication date (electronic) : 2024 July 2
doi : https://doi.org/10.7874/jao.2023.00346
1Physiotherapist (Independent Researcher), Paris, France
2Aix-Marseille University, Marseille, France
3Neurosciences Department, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Paris, France
Address for correspondence Michel Lacour, PhD 21 Impasse des Vertus, 13710 Fuveau, France Tel +33-6-1247-1247 E-mail Michel.lacour0802@gmail.com
Received 2023 August 10; Revised 2023 October 17; Accepted 2023 October 20.

Abstract

Background and Objectives

Perception of verticality is clinically assessed using the subjective visual vertical (SVV), a test of the otolith system that consists of aligning a bar on the gravitational vertical in darkness. Patients with acute unilateral vestibulopathy (AUVP) show a systematic SVV bias toward the affected side, whichever the side of line orientation. Whether SVV estimates are symmetrical has not been investigated.

Subjects and Methods

This study included 10 patients with AUVP (vestibular neuritis) and 10 with BPPV (posterior semicircular canal). SVV measurements were made at two preset angles of line orientation (15° and 30°) toward the ipsilateral and contralateral sides, relative to the affected side.

Results

The results showed asymmetrical SVV estimates in the AUVP group, with significantly greater SVV errors for ipsilateral than contralateral line orientation, as well as for the preset angle of 30° compared to 15°. SVV estimates were significantly lower in patients with BPPV who also exhibited SVV asymmetry. SVV estimates remained unchanged just after the maneuver and were normalized some days later or after supplementary maneuvers.

Conclusions

SVV asymmetry should be routinely considered in the clinic. We recommend individually assessing ipsilateral and contralateral SVV and using at least two preset angles. This allows for a better assessment and diagnosis of otolith organ imbalance that can trigger chronic instability and dizziness. The contribution of neck afferents related to head position in space seems to be the main source of SVV asymmetry.

Introduction

The perception of verticality is essential for spatial orientation and postural stability [1]. The otolith organs of the labyrinth encode the linear accelerations and play a major role in the perception of gravity and verticality [2]. The subjective visual vertical (SVV) test has long been used to assess the otolith system and to provide an estimate of the perception of verticality in both healthy subjects [3] and patients with vestibular dysfunction [4]. The contribution of vision in gravitational orientation has also been recognized [5], and the role of visual orientation cues has been particularly emphasized. Assessment of the SVV with the rod and frame test showed that the visual vertical estimate was biased by the square frame orientation [6,7]. Moreover, receptors of the plantar sole and muscle proprioception of the head and body provide the central nervous system with relevant inputs for the perception of upright. Biases in upright estimate known as the Müller effect (or E-effect: [8]) and the Aubert effect (A-effect: [9]) were observed for small (<60°) versus larger (>60°) body tilts corresponding to overestimation (bias away from the body axis) versus underestimation (bias toward the body axis), respectively. Psychophysical experiments in unilateral vestibular loss patients showed SVV alterations in relation to body tilt orientation [10].

An internal estimation of verticality perception is generated by the central integration of otolith signals encoding head orientation in space with respect to gravity, body proprioception encoding body orientation in space, and neck proprioception encoding the relative position of head on body [11]. The Mittelstaedt’s model of upright orientation is based on the vectorial summation of the idiotropic vector (Z longitudinal axis) and the gravity vector (otolith organs: [12]). Bayesian models incorporate head orientation in space and eye in head position in a common reference frame to determine upright orientation. The cortical network involved in the perception of orientation lies at the temporo-parietal junction, in the posterior insular vestibular cortex [11,13-15]. Perception of upright is a high level neural process, alteration of which has been reported in various balance system disorders: Parkinson’s disease, vestibular neuritis, vestibular schwannoma, Pisa syndrome, aging, hydrocephalus, chronic neck pains, acute phase of Meniere’s disease, and multiple sclerosis [16].

The perception of orientation relative to gravity can be assessed by psychophysical tasks. The SVV test uses the adjustment protocol of a visual line in total darkness (or in the absence of visual orientation cues) to report perceived earth-vertical orientation. In healthy participants, the errors typically remain within 2° of earth vertical while SVV deviations to the affected side were found in patients with vestibular nerve section and vestibular neuritis [4,17-19]. The rate of SVV deviation progressively decreased over weeks or months due to the vestibular compensation process [18,20-23]. The SVV test is used in clinical routine to detect otolith imbalance [24-26]. Interestingly, patients with benign paroxismal positional vertigo (BPPV) also showed SVV deviation to the affected side [16,27] that could result from reduced otolith receptor function, as the consequence of the decreased density of the otoconia macular mass on the affected side [28].

Different parameters can affect the perception of verticality when the adjustment protocol of a visual line relative to the true gravitational vertical is used. An attraction bias to the line presentation side has been reported in both healthy subjects [3,29,30] and acute unilateral vestibulopathy (AUVP) patients [29,31], but the SVV estimate was not influenced by the deviation of the preset angle [30]. The viewing distance [32], the torsional position of the eyes [20], and the head/body lateral orientation [10] also influence the SVV estimate. The dynamic SVV test performed in the presence of a moving visual surround showed asymmetrical estimates of verticality in patients with unilateral peripheral vestibular loss, depending on the direction of the optokinetic flow [18]. To our knowledge, the static SVV estimates never reported such asymmetry. The reason is that clinicians always measured the mean bias estimated from data collected independently of the line orientation side and of the preset angle. The aim of the study was to determine whether AUVP and BPPV patients also exhibit asymmetrical values with the static SVV psychophysical test. This question has clinical and functional implications for otolith system diagnosis and vestibular rehabilitation therapy as well.

Subjects and Methods

Subjects

The study population comprised 10 patients with AUVP (vestibular neuritis) and 10 patients with BPPV.

Neurotological examination of the AUVP patients was done in one tertiary referral center (Centre d’Explorations Fonctionnelles Otoneurologiques, Paris: Dr. Michel Toupet) and in one ENT Department (Hôpital Lariboisière, Paris: Dr. Charlotte Hautefort). All AUVP patients exhibited the five main inclusion criteria [33]: acute onset of spinning vertigo, horizontal rotatory spontaneous nystagmus beating to the intact side, positive head impulse test (HIT) on the weaker side, nausea, and postural imbalance. Spontaneous nystagmus was observed with and without visual fixation using video nystagmography goggles (Framiral, Grasse, France). The neurotological examination included the caloric test, the HIT, and the head shaking test. The caloric test (30° and 44°) showed diminished responses on the hypofunction side. The pathological weaker side was also determined when using the angular vestibulo-ocular reflex (aVOR) gain during passive video HIT (vHIT Ulmer; Synapsis, Marseille, France). The inclusion criteria were aVOR gain below 0.70 and when the presence of overt/covert saccades. The three pairs of semicircular canals were tested in order to document the vestibular deficit and to determine which part of the vestibular nerve (superior branch, inferior branch, or both branches) was impaired. Among the 10 AUVP patients, 9 had pathological HIT responses to horizontal canal test and vertical anterior canal test (superior barnch) while one had pathological HIT responses to horizontal, anterior and posterior canal tests. The ocular and colic vestibular evoked myogenic potentials (oVEMPs and cVEMPs) were not systematically performed. One patient among the 5 patients tested showed absent oVEMPs and cVMPs on the hypofunction side and normal responses on the healthy side while the remaining four patients showed abolished oVEMPs only on the hypofunction side. The patients’ history was done by one author (S.H.) who also preformed the SVV test.

The study population comprised 7 women and 3 men with a mean age of 51.5 years (range: 24–72 years), and included patients whose initial visit took place on average 8.9 days after symptoms onset (range: 5–21 days). The AUVP affected the left side in 6 patients and the right side in 4 patients. The characteristics of the AUVP patients are summarized in the Table 1.

Characteristics of the AUVP and BPPV patients

Clinical examination and patients’ history of the BPPV population were done by one author (S.H.) in her office (Paris). Out of 45 BPPV patients examined over the last 2 years, 10 had abnormal SVV values, i.e., more than 2° of mean deviation from the gravitational vertical. Those 10 patients reported acute, brief, and rotatory vertigo attacks when they changed their head position in space. Loss of balance and unstable gait were also frequently reported by the patients both during the vertigo attack and between the attacks. The Fukuda test was first performed to look at the body deviation, a postural parameter often associated with the weaker vestibular side, and the test of Halmagyi was used to check the absence of overt saccades. Induction of the positional nystagmus was performed by subjecting the patients to the right and left Dix-Hallpike diagnostic maneuvers. The type of BPPV was identified by the patterns of nystagmus induced by the positional maneuver. Eight among the 10 recruited patients received a diagnosis of unilateral posterior semicircular canal BPPV on the right side, and 2 showed a geotropic horizontal semicircular canal BPPV on the left side. The mean age of the patients (7 women and 3 men) was 61.2 years (range: 42–87 years) (Table 1). Epley’s maneuver was the canalith repositioning maneuver (CRM) used to treat the posterior canal BPPV while the barbecue maneuver was used to treat the geotropic horizontal canal BPPV.

Written informed consent to participate had been obtained for each patient. The investigation was performed according to the Helsinki Declaration and the experimental protocol was approved by the local ethics committee (CCPPRB Marseille).

SVV test

The SVV test was performed with the Framiral system (Grasse, France) which uses the adjustment protocol to assess the perceived vertical. The SVV protocol consisted of projecting a red fluorescent line, 30 cm long, on a white screen located 1.5 m in front of the patient. All patients were tested in standing position and wore goggles that limited their visual field to ±15° from central vision. This restriction device has the advantage of doing the SVV test in a non-dark room and of totally eliminating all visual orientation cues. The patients were asked to keep their head straight, to focus their attention to the line, and to adjust the line orientation to their own perception of verticality.

The line was pseudo-randomly oriented to the clockwise and the counterclockwise directions with preset angles of 15° and 30° from the vertical. The line was rotated with a velocity of 2° per second, and the patients were asked to indicate verbally when they perceived the line orientation as being vertical. To compare the patients with right and left unilateral vestibular deficits, and by convention, the SVV estimates were positively quoted when they were on the same side as the vestibular loss. Ten measurements were done, beginning with a first line deviation of 15° of preset angle on the left side (ipsilateral side for a patient with a left vestibular deficit, contralateral side for a patient with a right vestibular deficit) followed by the same deviation on the right side. A couple of line deviation to 30° of preset angle was then performed on both sides, and the same deviations with 15° and 30° were repeated on both sides. The patients were instructed to close their eyes between each trial, i.e., in the time period during which the software automatically changed the line orientation to the next position. The SVV estimates for the ipsilateral and contralateral line orientations were automatically recorded. The whole SVV test duration was around 5 minutes.

In the BPPV patients, the SVV was first evaluated before the diagnostic maneuver, and re-evaluated 15–30 minutes after the liberatory maneuver, and 3–4 days later for verification.

Statistical analysis

The mean value and the variance of the SVV estimates were calculated separately in the AUVP and the BPPV patients. The SVV values were first calculated for the whole of the data, independently of the line orientation side (ipsilateral vs. contralateral) and of the amplitude of the preset angle (15° and 30°). The SVV estimates were thereafter calculated as a function of the side (ipsilateral vs. contralateral) and amplitude (15° vs. 30°) of the preset angle.

Considering the small size of our two groups of patients and the distribution of the SVV in each group that did not follow a normal Gaussian law, non-parametric tests were used for the statistical analysis of the data. Comparisons were done with the Mann–Whitney U test and the Wilcoxon signed rank test (StateView II; SAS Software Inc., Cary, NC, USA). Results were considered significant for p<0.05.

Results

SVV in AUVP

The SVV bias was always on the side of the weaker side in the AUVP patients. The mean SVV estimate was calculated by pooling all the data recorded with ipsilateral and contralateral line orientations with respect to the hypofunction side, at the two preset angles of 15° and 30°. The mean bias in the AUVP group was 7.91°±4.2° (Fig. 1). Clinicians refer to this mean value to determine whether the patient’s SVV was pathologic or not, i.e., within or outside the normality range (±2°).

Fig. 1.

Subjective visual vertical (SVV) depends on the side of line orientation in the AUVP patients. The mean SVV estimates (±SD) are plotted as a function of the side of the line orientation with respect to the affected side: ipsilateral side and contralateral side. The assessment of the SVV in clinical routine pools together all the ipsilateral and contralateral measurements is shown for comparison. The horizontal grey area represents the normal range of the SVV in healthy subjects. Asymmetrical SVV values are seen with significantly higher estimates for line orientation to the ipsilateral side. *p<0.05; **p<0.01. AUVP, acute unilateral vestibulopathy.

When the data were pooled as a function of the side of line orientation (ipsilateral vs. contralateral), the results showed significantly different SVV estimates. The mean SVV bias was higher with ipsilateral line orientations (9.49°±3.9°) compared to contralateral line orientations (7.03°±3.8°; p<0.01), and higher compared to the mean bias including the ipsilateral and contralateral values (p<0.05).

Fig. 2 illustrates the mean SVV biases as a function of both the line preset angle (15° vs. 30°) and the side of the vestibular weakness (ipsilateral vs. contralateral). The data showed significantly higher SVV estimates with 30° of preset angle compared to 15° for ipsilateral line orientations (11.1°±4.1° vs. 8.3°±3.9°; p<0.01), but not for contralateral line orientations (p=0.30). Significant differences were also found between ipsilateral and contralateral 30° preset angles (11.1°±4.1° vs. 7.4±4.0; p<0.01), but not for ipsilateral and contralateral 15° preset angle (p=0.72). The mean SVV bias was significantly higher at 30° of preset angle irrespective of the side of line orientation compared to 15° of preset angle, but the difference was not significant (9.3°±4.5° vs. 7.7°±3.4°; p<0.04).

Fig. 2.

Subjective visual vertical (SVV) depends on the side and preset angle of line orientation in the AUVP patients. The boxplots show the median with the 1st and 3rd quartiles, and whiskers indicate the minimum and maximum SVV values. Significantly higher SVV values were recorded for 30° deviation of the line to the ipsilateral side compared to the contralateral side and the ipsilateral line deviation at 15°. The horizontal grey area indicates the non-pathological SVV range. **p<0.01. AUVP, acute unilateral vestibulopathy.

Table 2 illustrates the raw data as the mean (±standard deviation) of the SVV estimates for the ipsilateral, contralateral, and ipsilateral+contralateral 15° and 30° preset angles.

Modifications of the SVV in the acute and chronic unilateral vestibular patients

Supplementary data recorded in three chronic AUVP patients still complaining of instability and dizziness 1 year after onset of their symptoms also showed SVV asymmetrical values (Table 2). At 30° of preset angle, for instance, the mean values were 4.2°±0.8° and 2.2°±1.1° for ipsilateral and contralateral line orientations, respectively (p<0.001). These values were significantly lower than those recorded acutely after AUVP (p<0.001).

SVV in BPPV patients

The data collected in the BPPV patients with altered SVV showed a pattern of SVV deviations very similar to that observed in the AUVP patients. The main difference was the smaller SVV bias found in the VPPB BPPV patients, comparable to those found in chronic AUVP patients.

Significantly higher mean SVV errors were observed with 30° of preset angle compared to 15° of preset angle for ipsilateral line orientations (5.8°±2.8° vs. 4.0°±2.2°; p<0.05), and between 30° of preset angle to the ipsilateral side compared to the contralateral side (5.8°±2.8° vs. 3.4°±2.3°; p<0.01) (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3.

Subjective visual vertical (SVV) depends on the side and preset angle of line orientation in the BPPV patients. The boxplots illustrate the median with the 1st and 3rd quartiles, and whiskers indicate the minimum and maximum SVV values. The SVV estimates are lower compared to the AUVP patients, but similar asymmetrical values were seen depending on the side and preset angle of line orientation. Higher SVV deviations were recorded for ipsilateral 30° line orientation compared to contralateral 30° orientation and 15° ipsilateral line orientation. The horizontal grey area indicates the non-pathological SVV range. *p<0.05; **p<0.01. BPPV, benign paroxysmal positional vertigo; AUVP, acute unilateral vestibulopathy.

After the successful CRM, patients were asked to rest a few minutes before they were subjected to a second SVV test. The SVV bias remained was generally unchanged during this second test but the standard deviation of the estimates was increased, suggesting a less accurate precision of the perceived vertical after the CRM. The mean values recorded at 15° of preset angle on the ipsilateral side were 4.0°±2.2° before CRM and 4.16±3.2 after CRM, 5.8°±2.8° and 5.2°±3.9° for the pre-and post-CRMs, respectively, at 30° of preset angle on the same side. The SVV estimates returning within the ±2° normal range were found later on, when the patients were re-subjected to a control SVV test. Seven among the ten BPPV patients showed non-pathological SVV values when retested 3 days after the first CRM whereas supplementary maneuvers were needed in the remaining three patients to regain SVV values in the non-pathological range (2 CRMs in one patient and 3 CRMs in two patients).

Fig. 4 illustrates the mean SVV estimates from a 49-year-old man with a BPPV of the right posterior semicircular canal who was examined before and after the first successful CRM, and 3 days after. The other BPPV patients showed similar results after the first maneuver but they needed 2 or 3 supplementary maneuvers to regain non-pathological SVV values.

Fig. 4.

Effect of the CRM on the SVV estimates. Data was recorded in the case of a 49-year-old man with BPPV of the right semicircular posterior canal. The SVV was measured during the first examination of the patient, before and after the CRM (Epley’s maneuver). The SVV estimates were not improved despite the successful maneuver when the second test was done a short time after (30–60 minutes), but the SVV was normalized 3 days later in this patient. Data was recorded for ipsilateral and contralateral 15° line orientations. The horizontal grey area indicates the non-pathological SVV range. ***p<0.001. CRM, canalith repositioning maneuver; SVV, subjective visual vertical; BPPV, benign paroxysmal positional vertigo.

Discussion

Taken together, the study showed that AUVP patients 1) had systematic SVV deviations to the hypofunction side, a result in agreement with the literature, and 2) showed asymmetrical SVV deviations depending on both the side of the line orientation and the preset angle deviation, a new and original result. Our data extends the SVV asymmetry to our BPPV patients who exhibited deviations to their affected side. What is the cause of the static SVV asymmetry, and what are the functional implications for vestibular rehabilitation are the two main questions at the heart of this Discussion section.

SVV deviation, ocular torsional position and head orientation in space

The ocular tilt reaction described in AUVP patients combines head tilt, ocular cyclotorsion, skew deviation and tilt of the SVV toward the affected side, and result from the imbalance of the otolith organs [20,24]. According to Wade and Curthoys [34], the SVV bias would be caused by the ocular torsional position. Their conclusion is based on the close correlation (r=0.95) between the amplitude of the cyclotorsion and the SVV bias. We also reported such a correlation in Meniere’s disease patients after a curative unilateral vestibular neurectomy [35]. There is however no relationship of proportionality between these two parameters, and lesions of the thalamo-cortical vestibular pathways [13] and cortical lesions after stroke [36,37] induce SVV deviations without cyclotorsion, recent works clearly demonstrated the lack of causal relationship between SVV and cyclotorsion [11,38,39]. We did not record the torsional position of the AUVP patients in the present study. However, our BPPV patients did not show any cyclotorsion and they exhibited similar SVV biases, a result that does not support the eye cyclotorsion hypothesis.

The dynamic SVV recorded with a moving visual background elicits an optokinetic stimulation with long lasting asymmetrical effects after unilateral vestibular loss [18]. The optokinetic-induced circular vection produced lower dynamic SVV changes when the optokinetic flow was directed to the intact side compared to optokinetic flow directed to the diseased side. The low rotational velocity of the visual line (2°/s) used in our protocol could induce similar effects. The visual (entrainment) effect [40] consists of the entrainment of the ocular torsion by a visual line rotating around the line of sight in the direction of the line rotation. The entrainment of the ocular torsion is low in gain, but repeatable and reliable among subjects, with however considerable individual differences between subjects. If present, a counterclockwise line rotation for a preset angle on the right hypofunction side, for example, should produce an ocular torsion entrainment in the opposite direction with respect to the cyclotorsion resulting from the otolith imbalance. And the SVV bias should be reduced (and vice versa for a clockwise rotation and preset angle oriented on the intact side). Our data showed opposite results: the static SVV was significantly increased with preset angles on the weaker side and the line moving to the intact side. This second argument still does not support the contribution of eye position signals to the SVV asymmetry.

Modifications of the SVV perception in relation to body tilt and head orientation in space have been described in healthy subjects (cf. the A- and E-effects) and in vestibular pathology [10]. Moreover, a correlation between the SVV bias and the head tilt has also been reported [21]. Accuracy and precision of the SVV were worse with the head tilted compared to the head aligned with gravity [41], and SVV imprecision was increased in vestibular patients with chronic instability. An interesting observation in our AUVP patients was the presence of head tilt (or head and body tilt) to the affected side when performing the SVV test while they showed head-longitudinal axis aligned with gravity in the light, just after removing the goggles that suppressed all the visual orientation cues. Assuming that head-based graviceptive signals provide the major input for internal estimates of visual vertical [41], and that the E-effect is absent or reduced in patients with peripheral unilateral vestibular loss, the neck afferents could play a predominant role in the SVV asymmetry. The contribution of neck muscle proprioception was indeed increased as early as one week in unilateral vestibular lesioned monkeys. The role of neck afferents in the compensation of unilateral vestibular deficits is still under debates, and further investigations are needed to clarify this point.

Finally, the SVV asymmetry could result from the right hemispheric dominance in right-handed subjects [42]. It was also reported that patients with right vestibular deficit compensated more slowly than patients with left vestibular dysfunction [43]. All the AUVP and BPPV patients included in our study had a right lateralization. Brain asymmetry regarding vestibular dominance at the cortical level is another hypothesis to account for the SVV asymmetry.

Functional implications for vestibular rehabilitation

A new and original result in the present study is the asymmetry of the static SVV. Most of the SVV estimates are based on software that calculates the mean SVV value from one or two preset angles of a line pseudo-randomly oriented to either side. In the absence of oVEMPs and cVEMPs, clinicians refer to this mean value to determine an otolith imbalance in AUVP patients. They base their diagnostic on an SVV estimate that considers neither the amplitude of the preset angle nor the side to which the line is oriented. In such conditions, the mean estimate can be within the normal range (<2°) in spite of pathological SVV values on one side. BPPV patients with pathological SVV estimates do not constitute the majority of the patients treated with the CRM. The asymmetrical SVV values found in our small BPPV population point to an otolith system imbalance that could not be revealed with SVV estimates pooling all the data.

Of key significance for patients, our data show the potential of this new assessment of the SVV taking into account the side and the amplitude of the preset angle. This method avoids underestimation of the otolith dysfunction and its consequences in term of instability, impairment of the patient’s quality of life, and possible development of chronic dizziness. Pathologies such as BPPV, AUVP, and vestibular migraine have been reported as prior triggers to the onset of persistent postural perceptive dizziness (PPPD: [44,45]). It is therefore recommended to abandon the mean SVV value and to determine the SVV errors for each side, ipsilaterally and contralaterally with respect to the disease side, and to use at least two different preset angles in the low range 10°–60° for which the E-effect can be modified after unilateral vestibular loss.

Limitations of the study

Our results will have to be confirmed on a broader sample of AUVP and BPPV patients. Unpublished data from 42 AUVP patients included in another paper [23] confirm however the SVV asymmetry found in the present study. All the BPPV patients do not show SVV deviations to their diseased side, and it is necessary to increase the size of this population not only to confirm our data, but also to determine when the patients should be retested and how many repositioning maneuvers have to be done to normalize the SVV. A better understanding of the mechanisms responsible for the SVV asymmetry should include measurements of the eye cyclotorsion, eye countercyclotorsion, and head position in space to determine the contribution of eye position signals and neck afferents.

Notes

Conflicts of Interest

The authors have no financial conflicts of interest.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: Souad Haijoub, Michel Lacour. Data curation: Michel Lacour. Formal analysis: Michel Lacour. Funding acquisition: Souad Haijoub. Investigation: Souad Haijoub. Methodology: Souad Haijoub, Michel Lacour. Software: Michel Lacour. Supervision: Michel Lacour. Validation: Michel Lacour. Visualization: Souad Haijoub, Michel Lacour. Writing—original draft: Michel Lacour. Writing—review & editing: Souad Haijoub, Michel Lacour. Approval of final manuscript: Souad Haijoub, Michel Lacour.

Funding Statement

None

Acknowledgements

The autors wish to thank Dr. Charlotte Hautefort and Dr. Michel Toupet for the neurotological examination of the patients, and Mr. Steve Edmondson for correcting the English language.

References

1. Dakin CJ, Rosenberg A. Gravity estimation and verticality perception. Handb Clin Neurol 2018;159:43–59.
2. Bronstein AM. The interaction of otolith and proprioceptive information in the perception of verticality. The effects of labyrinthine and CNS disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1999;871:324–33.
3. Iranfar S, Jalilvand Karimi L, Shaabani M, Sharifian Alborzi M, Akbarzadeh Baghban A. Subjective visual vertical test: the effect of preset angle and visual background. J Rehab Med 2013;2:47–54.
4. Böhmer A, Rickenmann J. The subjective visual vertical as a clinical parameter of vestibular function in peripheral vestibular diseases. J Vestib Res 1995;5:35–45.
5. Dichgans J, Brandt T, Held R. The role of vision in gravitational orientation. Fortschr Zool 1975;23:255–63.
6. Mittelstaedt H. The subjective vertical as a function of visual and extraretinal cues. Acta Psychol (Amst) 1986;63:63–85.
7. Kupferberg A, Glasauer S, Stein A, Brandt T. Influence of uninformative visual cues on gravity perception. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2009;1164:403–5.
8. Yelnik AP, Lebreton FO, Bonan IV, Colle FM, Meurin FA, Guichard JP, et al. Perception of verticality after recent cerebral hemispheric stroke. Stroke 2002;33:2247–53.
9. Guerraz M, Poquin D, Luyat M, Ohlmann T. Head orientation involvement in assessment of the subjective vertical during whole body tilt. Percept Mot Skills 1998;87:643–8.
10. Lopez C, Lacour M, Léonard J, Magnan J, Borel L. How body position changes visual vertical perception after unilateral vestibular loss. Neuropsychologia 2008;46:2435–40.
11. Kheradmand A, Winnick A. Perception of upright: multisensory convergence and the role of temporo-parietal cortex. Front Neurol 2017;8:552.
12. Mittelstaedt H. A new solution to the problem of the subjective vertical. Naturwissenschaften 1983;70:272–81.
13. Brandt T, Dieterich M, Danek A. Vestibular cortex lesions affect the perception of verticality. Ann Neurol 1994;35:403–12.
14. Lopez C, Blanke O. The thalamocortical vestibular system in animals and humans. Brain Res Rev 2011;67:119–46.
15. Baier B, Zu Eulenburg P, Best C, Geber C, Müller-Forell W, Birklein F, et al. Posterior insular cortex – a site of vestibular–somatosensory interaction? Brain Behav 2013;3:519–24.
16. Bolandi Shirejini M, Farahani A, Nazeri A. The application of subjective visual vertical in balance system disorders. Aud Vestib Res 2018;27:1–11.
17. Vibert D, Häusler R. Long-term evolution of subjective visual vertical after vestibular neurectomy and labyrinthectomy. Acta Otolaryngol 2000;120:620–2.
18. Lopez C, Lacour M, Ahmadi AE, Magnan J, Borel L. Changes of visual vertical perception: a long-term sign of unilateral and bilateral vestibular loss. Neuropsychologia 2007;45:2025–37.
19. Tarnutzer AA, Bockisch C, Straumann D, Olasagasti I. Gravity dependence of subjective visual vertical variability. J Neurophysiol 2009;102:1657–71.
20. Curthoys IS, Dai MJ, Halmagyi GM. Human ocular torsional position before and after unilateral vestibular neurectomy. Exp Brain Res 1991;85:218–25.
21. Redon C, Lopez C, Bernard-Demanze L, Dumitrescu M, Magnan J, Lacour M, et al. Betahistine treatment improves the recovery of static symptoms in patients with unilateral vestibular loss. J Clin Pharmacol 2011;51:538–48.
22. Lacour M, Thiry A, Tardivet L. Two conditions to fully recover dynamic canal function in unilateral peripheral vestibular hypofunction patients. J Vestib Res 2021;31:407–21.
23. Lacour M, Lopez C, Thiry A, Tardivet L. Vestibular rehabilitation improves spontaneous nystagmus normalization in patients with acute unilateral vestibulopathy. Front Rehabil Sci 2023;4:1122301.
24. Halmagyi GM, Gresty MA, Gibson WP. Ocular tilt reaction with peripheral vestibular lesion. Ann Neurol 1979;6:80–3.
25. Fetter M. Assessing vestibular function: which tests, when? J Neurol 2000;247:335–42.
26. Clarke AH, Schönfeld U, Helling K. Unilateral examination of utricle and saccule function. J Vestib Res 2003;13:215–25.
27. Gall RM, Ireland DJ, Robertson DD. Subjective visual vertical in patients with benign paroxysmal positional vertigo. J Otolaryngol 1999;28:162–5.
28. Faralli M, Manzari L, Panichi R, Botti F, Ricci G, Longari F, et al. Subjective visual vertical before and after treatment of a BPPV episode. Auris Nasus Larynx 2011;38:307–11.
29. Toupet M, Van Nechel C, Grayeli AB. Subjective visual vertical tilt attraction to the side of rod presentation: effects of age, sex, and vestibular disorders. Otol Neurotol 2015;36:1074–80.
30. Venhovens J, Meulstee J, Verhagen WI. Static subjective visual vertical in healthy volunteers: the effects of different preset angle deviations and test-retest variability. Neuroophthalmology 2016;40:113–9.
31. Pagarkar W, Bamiou DE, Ridout D, Luxon LM. Subjective visual vertical and horizontal: effect of the preset angle. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 2008;134:394–401.
32. Kheradmand A, Gonzalez G, Otero-Millan J, Lasker A. Visual perception of upright: head tilt, visual errors and viewing eye. J Vestib Res 2016;25:201–9.
33. Strupp M, Magnusson M. Acute unilateral vestibulopathy. Neurol Clin 2015;33:669–85.
34. Wade SW, Curthoys IS. The effect of ocular torsional position on perception of the roll-tilt of visual stimuli. Vision Res 1997;37:1071–8.
35. Lopez C, Borel L, Magnan J, Lacour M. Torsional optokinetic nystagmus after unilateral vestibular loss: asymmetry and compensation. Brain 2005;128:1511–24.
36. Yelnik AP, Lebreton FO, Bonan IV, Colle FM, Meurin FA, Guichard JP, et al. Perception of verticality after recent cerebral hemispheric stroke. Stroke 2002;33:2247–53.
37. Saj A, Borel L, Honoré J. Functional neuroanatomy of vertical visual perception in humans. Front Neurol 2019;10:142.
38. Tarnutzer AA, Bockisch CJ, Straumann D. Visually guided adjustments of body posture in the roll plane. Exp Brain Res 2013;227:111–20.
39. Otero-Millan J, Kheradmand A. Upright perception and ocular torsion change independently during head tilt. Front Hum Neurosci 2016;10:573.
40. Mezey LE, Curthoys IS, Burgess AM, Goonetilleke SC, MacDougall HG. Changes in ocular torsion position produced by a single visual line rotating around the line of sight--visual “entrainment” of ocular torsion. Vision Res 2004;44:397–406.
41. Tarnutzer AA, Bockisch CJ, Straumann D. Roll-dependent modulation of the subjective visual vertical: contributions of head- and trunkbased signals. J Neurophysiol 2010;103:934–41.
42. Dieterich M, Bense S, Lutz S, Drzezga A, Stephan T, Bartenstein P, et al. Dominance for vestibular cortical function in the non-dominant hemisphere. Cereb Cortex 2003;13:994–1007.
43. Toupet M, Van Nechel C, Bozorg Grayeli A. Influence of body laterality on recovery from subjective visual vertical tilt after vestibular neuritis. Audiol Neurootol 2014;19:248–55.
44. Habs M, Strobl R, Grill E, Dieterich M, Becker-Bense S. Primary or secondary chronic functional dizziness: does it make a difference? A DizzyReg study in 356 patients. J Neurol 2020;267(Suppl 1):212–22.
45. Staab JP, Eckhardt-Henn A, Horii A, Jacob R, Strupp M, Brandt T, et al. Diagnostic criteria for persistent postural-perceptual dizziness (PPPD): consensus document of the committee for the classification of vestibular disorders of the Bárány society. J Vestib Res 2017;27:191–208.

Article information Continued

Fig. 1.

Subjective visual vertical (SVV) depends on the side of line orientation in the AUVP patients. The mean SVV estimates (±SD) are plotted as a function of the side of the line orientation with respect to the affected side: ipsilateral side and contralateral side. The assessment of the SVV in clinical routine pools together all the ipsilateral and contralateral measurements is shown for comparison. The horizontal grey area represents the normal range of the SVV in healthy subjects. Asymmetrical SVV values are seen with significantly higher estimates for line orientation to the ipsilateral side. *p<0.05; **p<0.01. AUVP, acute unilateral vestibulopathy.

Fig. 2.

Subjective visual vertical (SVV) depends on the side and preset angle of line orientation in the AUVP patients. The boxplots show the median with the 1st and 3rd quartiles, and whiskers indicate the minimum and maximum SVV values. Significantly higher SVV values were recorded for 30° deviation of the line to the ipsilateral side compared to the contralateral side and the ipsilateral line deviation at 15°. The horizontal grey area indicates the non-pathological SVV range. **p<0.01. AUVP, acute unilateral vestibulopathy.

Fig. 3.

Subjective visual vertical (SVV) depends on the side and preset angle of line orientation in the BPPV patients. The boxplots illustrate the median with the 1st and 3rd quartiles, and whiskers indicate the minimum and maximum SVV values. The SVV estimates are lower compared to the AUVP patients, but similar asymmetrical values were seen depending on the side and preset angle of line orientation. Higher SVV deviations were recorded for ipsilateral 30° line orientation compared to contralateral 30° orientation and 15° ipsilateral line orientation. The horizontal grey area indicates the non-pathological SVV range. *p<0.05; **p<0.01. BPPV, benign paroxysmal positional vertigo; AUVP, acute unilateral vestibulopathy.

Fig. 4.

Effect of the CRM on the SVV estimates. Data was recorded in the case of a 49-year-old man with BPPV of the right semicircular posterior canal. The SVV was measured during the first examination of the patient, before and after the CRM (Epley’s maneuver). The SVV estimates were not improved despite the successful maneuver when the second test was done a short time after (30–60 minutes), but the SVV was normalized 3 days later in this patient. Data was recorded for ipsilateral and contralateral 15° line orientations. The horizontal grey area indicates the non-pathological SVV range. ***p<0.001. CRM, canalith repositioning maneuver; SVV, subjective visual vertical; BPPV, benign paroxysmal positional vertigo.

Table 1.

Characteristics of the AUVP and BPPV patients

Characteristic AUVP patients (n=10) BPPV patients (n=10)
Age (yr) 51.5 (24-72) 61.2 (42-87)
Sex
 Females 7 7
 Males 3 3
Time from symptoms onset (day) 8.9 (5-21) 7.5 (3-18)
Disease side
 Right 4 8 (posterior SCC)
 Left 6 2 (lateral SCC)

Values are presented as mean (ranges) or numbers only. AUVP, acute unilateral vestibular pathology; BPPV, benign paroxysmal positional vertigo; SCC, semicircular canal

Table 2.

Modifications of the SVV in the acute and chronic unilateral vestibular patients

Side and preset angle SVV (°)
Acute unilateral vestibular patients (n=10) Chronic unilateral vestivular patients (n=3)
Ipsilateral 15° 8.3±2.9 3.1±1.5
Ipsilateral 30° 11.1±4.1* 4.2±0.8
Contralateral 15° 7.0±3.5 2.2±1.4
Contralateral 30° 7.4±4.0 2.2±1.1
Ipsilateral+contralateral 15° 7.7±3.4 2.5±1.4
Ipsilateral+contralateral 30° 9.4±4.5 3.2±1.3

Values are presented as mean±standard deviation.

*

p=0.006 (vs. ipsilateral 15°) and p=0.003 (vs. contralateral 30°);

p=0.05 (vs. ipsilateral 15°) and p=0.001 (vs. contralateral 30°).

SVV, subjective visual vertical